I'm not a huge fan of Stephen Pinker's psychology, but he is a solid
writer, and I respect his perspective on many subjects. So when he wrote
a Chronicle article on "
Why Academics Stink at Writing" I took notice. The article starts by considering, and rejecting several suggestions for why academic writing is so bad:
- Bad writing is there deliberately, to stop normal people from realizing scholars are talking about nothing.
- Bad writing cannot be avoided, because the topics of discussion are so complex.
- Bad writing is virtually required by reviewers and editors, who will not accept papers written in more straightforward manners.
These
things happen, but apply to a very small percentage of published work,
Pinker claims. Instead, Pinker suggests that academic writing is bad
because it tries to mix writing styles, and authors become muddled about
the audience and its desires. As he puts it:
Most
academic writing, in contrast, is a blend of two styles. The first is
practical style, in which the writer’s goal is to satisfy a reader’s
need for a particular kind of information, and the form of the
communication falls into a fixed template, such as the five-paragraph
student essay or the standardized structure of a scientific article. The
second is a style that Thomas and Turner call self-conscious,
relativistic, ironic, or postmodern, in which "the writer’s chief, if
unstated, concern is to escape being convicted of philosophical naïveté
about his own enterprise."
With this perspective in
mind, Pinker argues that much bad writing in academia is the result of
"agonizing self-consciousness". This leads to too much meta-discussion,
and leads academics to lose the balance between their role as
communicators of knowledge vs. their role as members of a profession
with its own internal norms and mores. There are many good criticisms of
common phrases used by academics, which weaken their writing, and bad
habits, such as the misuse of scare quotes. He goes on to talk about how
certain cognitive processes (chunking, functional fixity, and the curse
of knowledge) make it hard for authors to realize what will make sense
to their readers. And he ends with a discussion about how few obvious
incentives there are for academics to write well. For the most part I
nodded in agreement, and thought about making some minor tweaks to a few
papers that are in the pipeline. However, there were two points that
made me uneasy.
First, Pinker criticizes "apologizing", such as
when authors say that the topics they are about to write on are
"extremely complex." I can see how this can be inappropriate in some
circumstances, but I think the audience needs to be considered. Many of
the things I write about are not subjects that others think about much,
and when others do think about those subjects, they tend to think things
are very simple. In that context, when I use the language Pinker is
criticizing, it is because I am informing the reader that their initial
views might be mistaken. For example, the types of psychological
questions you can ask using a rat, in a box with a level and a few
lights,
are quite complex. Many psychology students and even
many psychology professors (nevertheless members of the general public)
do not believe that assertion, until they have learned quite a bit about
the amazing studies that people have done.
Second, Pinker
criticizes authors who "hedge" their statements, rather than relying on
the reader to be charitable. This criticism baffled me. Certainly it is
possible to over-hedge, but Pinker lives in a world full of
non-charitable readers. I cannot understand his position except as a
weird statement of elitism: He is too influential to be taken down by
minor nit-picking, so he assumes all academics have the luxury of
ignoring it as well. In my world, there is a big difference between
making a claim such as "Perception is accurate" and saying "For the most
part, perception is accurate." Depending on the context, a paper could
easily get rejected for the hedged sentence, or get rejected for the
non-hedged sentence. --- In fairness, Pinker acknowledges that some
hedging may be necessary, but argues that skilled writers use it
cautiously, rather than as a "tick." Alas, I'm not sure
that hedge is sufficient to convey the reality; most academic authors face extremely ungenerous gatekeepers.
It
is also interesting to note how often Pinker cannot resist the urge to
be clever, inserting semi-jokes, at the expense of clarity. I do that
too, but I am not sure I would do it so much in a piece specifically
about clear writing. It makes his article half-way between something
amazingly clear, like
Elements of Style, and joking self-aware rule lists, with entries such as "Preposition are not things to end a sentence with."
Overall, however, very good, and recommended reading.